Gross, A. G. (1996). The arrangement of a scientific paper. In The Rhetoric of Science. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. p. 85-96.
Induction is a fallacy. Gross argues that we recognize causes based on effects. Thus, inductive reasoning becomes a circular argument in that in one case, a certain effect indicates a certain cause; therefore, one can continue to expect that cause to precede a similar effect (assuming uniformity).
I know... it makes my head spin too. Here's the example that Gross uses: Everyday a man goes to feed a chicken. The chicken correctly argues that if the man cares for the chicken, then the man will feed the chicken. The problem however, lies in assuming that because the man feeds the chicken, he cares for it. One day, the man will not come to feed the chicken, but to kill it for dinner.
So, what does this have to do with scienctific writing and the scientific paper? Everything. Last post, I talked about Medawar's assertion that the scientific paper is fraudulent and misleading. His argument? Scientists have an agenda of sorts, we don't actually use pure induction, but rather plan and execute experiments and observations with a hypothesis in mind.
Gross addresses this very argument in this chapter however, proposing that whatever developments have been made in the philosophy of science, the structure of the scientific article will remain the same to uphold the myth of induction. He bases this theory on that of Levi-Strauss, who analyzes myths as "a logical model capable of overcoming a [fundamental] contradiction." (Gross, 1996, p. 95).
What is the fundamental contradiction in science? Perhaps it is between theory and reality. Even so, I think I would describe it like this:
The inductive process tries to take little pieces of reality and put them together into new knowledge, whereas deduction begins with a series of simplified assumptions to create a "perfect world." Experiments are analogous to induction; the scientist is trying to isolate random facts to later reassemble. Theory is analogous to deduction; the theorist creates a scholarly model of the way the world should work. Bacon and Euclides are emblematic of these two realms and the scientific paper mediates between the two.
How does the scientific paper mediate? I argue that in reality, the scientific paper combines deduction and induction. Truthfully, this is what I've been teaching my students for the past few years. The introduction serves to establish context and explain the underlying assumptions and theories that frame the proposed experiments. Later, the discussion takes the myriad of results (isolated facts) and pulls them together into a solid argument. Deduction followed by induction.
According to Gross, the mediating nature of the scientific paper requires that the structure remain constant. Otherwise, the philosophy of science (we never really know) gets mixed up with the practice of science, and confidence in that practice is shaken.
Come to think of it... that's a pretty scary argument. Do we have too much confidence in that practice? Is this why many non-scientists distrust the practice of science - because of its ever-changing nature? In this light, perhaps we should reconsider Medawar's arguments.
Especially since scientists neither read nor write scientific articles in the order they are structured.
"Algae specialists, long near the bottom of the biology food chain, are becoming the rock stars."
Bourne, National Geographic, Oct. 2007
Bourne, National Geographic, Oct. 2007
Tuesday, January 24, 2012
Wednesday, January 18, 2012
Ludicrous Pretense in Scientific Papers: Medawar's Three Laws
Medawar, P. B. 1964. Is the scientific paper fraudulent? Saturday Review, 49:42-43.
1. The Law of Conservation of Information: Generalizations cannot contain more information than the sum of their foundations.
2. The Law of Entropy of Information: Orderly general statements do not somehow emerge from a disorderly array of facts.
3. The Law of Bias in Information: There is no such thing as unprejudiced observation.
While this is a sort of "tongue in cheek" summary of Medawar's paper (and one I think Medawar would appreciate, based on his writing) the concepts merit serious consideration.
Like Hoffmann, Medawar challenges the structure of the scientific paper as a misrepresentation of the nature of scientific thought. He even goes so far as to label the standard form fraudulent, a totally mistaken conception, and a travesty. According to Medawar, the "orthodox" IMRAD (Introduction, Methods, Results, And Discussion) format follows an inductive structure.
So, what's the problem with the inductive approach? Well, using induction, one cannot both discover and prove at the same time. Doing both requires the hypothetico-deductive interpretation. In other words, scientists generally have the end in mind when they begin an experiment. Hypotheses allow for focus in experimental design. Deduction gives us a framework or a lens through which to view data.
This is a HUGE difference between the physical sciences and the social sciences. From what I've learned so far, social scientists are all about identifying bias. They know its there, acknowledge it, and move on. I'm beginning to think we scientists are a repressed lot...
Anyway, Medawar's big take-home message here is this: Turn the scientific article upside down. Put your conclusions first and then set about showing support with the data.
I find this suggestion rather intriguing, especially recalling Chris Mooney's post, "Good communication is good scientific practice." He discusses the most important facets of communicating science to the public: put the bottom line up front.
In a response to last week's post, I talked about why I thought Hoffman's suggestions for change to the scientific paper were nigh impossible. Medawar's may be closer to our grasp. Indeed, some elements are already sporadically present. While the discussion remains at the end of the paper, it is occasionally combined with the results, and introductions are often direct in the intent of the researcher to apply biological principles to some ecological "problem" or at least identifies the theory through which the study finds its meaning.
I have to run off to do a class visit, ironically on how to write a scientific paper. I'll share my thoughts on this later.
1. The Law of Conservation of Information: Generalizations cannot contain more information than the sum of their foundations.
2. The Law of Entropy of Information: Orderly general statements do not somehow emerge from a disorderly array of facts.
3. The Law of Bias in Information: There is no such thing as unprejudiced observation.
While this is a sort of "tongue in cheek" summary of Medawar's paper (and one I think Medawar would appreciate, based on his writing) the concepts merit serious consideration.
Like Hoffmann, Medawar challenges the structure of the scientific paper as a misrepresentation of the nature of scientific thought. He even goes so far as to label the standard form fraudulent, a totally mistaken conception, and a travesty. According to Medawar, the "orthodox" IMRAD (Introduction, Methods, Results, And Discussion) format follows an inductive structure.
So, what's the problem with the inductive approach? Well, using induction, one cannot both discover and prove at the same time. Doing both requires the hypothetico-deductive interpretation. In other words, scientists generally have the end in mind when they begin an experiment. Hypotheses allow for focus in experimental design. Deduction gives us a framework or a lens through which to view data.
This is a HUGE difference between the physical sciences and the social sciences. From what I've learned so far, social scientists are all about identifying bias. They know its there, acknowledge it, and move on. I'm beginning to think we scientists are a repressed lot...
Anyway, Medawar's big take-home message here is this: Turn the scientific article upside down. Put your conclusions first and then set about showing support with the data.
I find this suggestion rather intriguing, especially recalling Chris Mooney's post, "Good communication is good scientific practice." He discusses the most important facets of communicating science to the public: put the bottom line up front.
In a response to last week's post, I talked about why I thought Hoffman's suggestions for change to the scientific paper were nigh impossible. Medawar's may be closer to our grasp. Indeed, some elements are already sporadically present. While the discussion remains at the end of the paper, it is occasionally combined with the results, and introductions are often direct in the intent of the researcher to apply biological principles to some ecological "problem" or at least identifies the theory through which the study finds its meaning.
I have to run off to do a class visit, ironically on how to write a scientific paper. I'll share my thoughts on this later.
Saturday, January 7, 2012
"The Language of My Science"
Hoffman, R. (1988). Under the surface of the chemical article. Angewandte Chemie International Edition in English, 27, 1593-1602.
In case you want to read it too. I recommend it.
Hoffman looks at the chemistry article through a non-science scholar's eyes and paints a beautiful picture of what chemistry (and science) really is, and how the reality of science can be obscured by the emotionless form of the scientific article.
My husband and I have a fundamental disagreement on the beauty of nature. He sees chaos and danger, while I see an amazing interconnected world in which every drop of water, every leaf, every living, moving thing is amazing. Except maybe spiders. I might agree with him on spiders.
Anyway, the way I see nature is why I became a scientist. The world is cool, and I wanted (and still want) to know more. My view of nature is why I am so driven to share with others. I want to make sure they're not missing out. I'm really not trying to be sappy and melodramatic here, but it is true. This world is crazy exciting. Going deeper just makes it better. Like ogres.
The thing is, most people don't see that kind of excitement when they read a scientific article. Most of the time, it is because it is not there. Science and science writing are supposed to be objective and unemotional. Hoffmann argues that because of this personal distance in our writing, scientists come off as cold and stoic, an automaton that doesn't make mistakes.
I agree with Hoffmann. I think that this image has done science a bad turn. When we remove humanity from science, science becomes this vague "thing" that does this or that. Thus, when some public scandal hits one scientist, it hits us all. But, who reads scientific articles anyway? Not laypersons (not usually, anyway. I do know a few.).
Hoffmann points out in this article that the current concise and objective form of the scientific article came about because of "Natural Philosophers" who pulled evidences from and described "Nature" without verifying what nature is actually like. Thus, a standard format requiring experimental evidence squelched the poets and sages. Has the squelching gone too far? Maybe. Scientists have a really hard time (in general) communicating with the outside world. Maybe we do need to reconsider how we communicate with one another.
I think perhaps that I should put on my history of science hat and think about this a different way. The point of the article is, that the scientific journal article never tells the whole story. It leaves out the real process - the trial and error, the frustration, the emotion, the fact that I had three months of sample data missing because I dropped and broke an entire tray of crucibles containing my samples. We might be able to understand the progress of the field by looking at trends in articles, but we'll never understand the progress of a single article by reading that particular article. And yet, this does not invalidate the progress of science (see Hoffmann's Personal View #6: As a system, science works).
There is a lot to say about this article, and I haven't even really expressed a kernel of what Hoffmann succeeds in saying. Perhaps the main take-home message is that the language and the form of the scientific article communicates more than we think and less than it should.
In case you want to read it too. I recommend it.
Hoffman looks at the chemistry article through a non-science scholar's eyes and paints a beautiful picture of what chemistry (and science) really is, and how the reality of science can be obscured by the emotionless form of the scientific article.
My husband and I have a fundamental disagreement on the beauty of nature. He sees chaos and danger, while I see an amazing interconnected world in which every drop of water, every leaf, every living, moving thing is amazing. Except maybe spiders. I might agree with him on spiders.
Anyway, the way I see nature is why I became a scientist. The world is cool, and I wanted (and still want) to know more. My view of nature is why I am so driven to share with others. I want to make sure they're not missing out. I'm really not trying to be sappy and melodramatic here, but it is true. This world is crazy exciting. Going deeper just makes it better. Like ogres.
The thing is, most people don't see that kind of excitement when they read a scientific article. Most of the time, it is because it is not there. Science and science writing are supposed to be objective and unemotional. Hoffmann argues that because of this personal distance in our writing, scientists come off as cold and stoic, an automaton that doesn't make mistakes.
I agree with Hoffmann. I think that this image has done science a bad turn. When we remove humanity from science, science becomes this vague "thing" that does this or that. Thus, when some public scandal hits one scientist, it hits us all. But, who reads scientific articles anyway? Not laypersons (not usually, anyway. I do know a few.).
Hoffmann points out in this article that the current concise and objective form of the scientific article came about because of "Natural Philosophers" who pulled evidences from and described "Nature" without verifying what nature is actually like. Thus, a standard format requiring experimental evidence squelched the poets and sages. Has the squelching gone too far? Maybe. Scientists have a really hard time (in general) communicating with the outside world. Maybe we do need to reconsider how we communicate with one another.
I think perhaps that I should put on my history of science hat and think about this a different way. The point of the article is, that the scientific journal article never tells the whole story. It leaves out the real process - the trial and error, the frustration, the emotion, the fact that I had three months of sample data missing because I dropped and broke an entire tray of crucibles containing my samples. We might be able to understand the progress of the field by looking at trends in articles, but we'll never understand the progress of a single article by reading that particular article. And yet, this does not invalidate the progress of science (see Hoffmann's Personal View #6: As a system, science works).
There is a lot to say about this article, and I haven't even really expressed a kernel of what Hoffmann succeeds in saying. Perhaps the main take-home message is that the language and the form of the scientific article communicates more than we think and less than it should.
Back!
For the scores of followers who were disappointed at my absence, the end of the semester included a huge workload, followed by a two-week trip to Maryland for the holidays.
I really like the part of Maryland where my parents live; it is kind of a low-key area, very local, and close to the quaint beaches and bays of the Chesapeake. One of my favorite aspects of the area are the fossil cliffs that erode away to reveal Miocene fossils, including scallops, barnacles, ray dental plates, and shark teeth. I have some great photos and even brought home a couple fossils from the Calvert Maritime Museum, home of Bubbles and Squeak, the river otters. It is also home to the Drum Point Lighthouse, the William B. Tennyson, and Mindy - an exhibits interpreter that works with the museum's outreach program. I got to "talk shop" with Mindy, which was really great - CMM has a distance learning outreach program that I'm hoping to check out sometime, because it sounds pretty cool.
I also got my husband to touch a sea star. No really. With one whole finger tip. His favorite part was the ray tank (no petting there) featuring butterfly rays, Atlantic rays, and skates.
Anyway, I wanted to come back and say hello, because it is about to get all crazy on the ol'blog! I am taking a History of Science course this semester, on the History of Science Writing and Rhetoric. Since it is an independent study, my instructor has asked me to post my responses to the readings on my blog!
Since I'm carrying a 15-credit load this semester (which for a graduate student is near ridiculous), I am going to try and do a lot of the reading and responding for this course up front, before the semester really gets going. With five units, and eight readings per unit, that means (hopefully) several posts per day!
One thing that I would love is feedback and discussion... so if you want to pass my blog on to others who might enjoy, I would really appreciate that.
Best!
PlanktonGirl
I really like the part of Maryland where my parents live; it is kind of a low-key area, very local, and close to the quaint beaches and bays of the Chesapeake. One of my favorite aspects of the area are the fossil cliffs that erode away to reveal Miocene fossils, including scallops, barnacles, ray dental plates, and shark teeth. I have some great photos and even brought home a couple fossils from the Calvert Maritime Museum, home of Bubbles and Squeak, the river otters. It is also home to the Drum Point Lighthouse, the William B. Tennyson, and Mindy - an exhibits interpreter that works with the museum's outreach program. I got to "talk shop" with Mindy, which was really great - CMM has a distance learning outreach program that I'm hoping to check out sometime, because it sounds pretty cool.
I also got my husband to touch a sea star. No really. With one whole finger tip. His favorite part was the ray tank (no petting there) featuring butterfly rays, Atlantic rays, and skates.
Anyway, I wanted to come back and say hello, because it is about to get all crazy on the ol'blog! I am taking a History of Science course this semester, on the History of Science Writing and Rhetoric. Since it is an independent study, my instructor has asked me to post my responses to the readings on my blog!
Since I'm carrying a 15-credit load this semester (which for a graduate student is near ridiculous), I am going to try and do a lot of the reading and responding for this course up front, before the semester really gets going. With five units, and eight readings per unit, that means (hopefully) several posts per day!
One thing that I would love is feedback and discussion... so if you want to pass my blog on to others who might enjoy, I would really appreciate that.
Best!
PlanktonGirl
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)